This has led researchers to classify the top-down attentional mod

This has led researchers to classify the top-down attentional modulation of visual neurons response into feature-based (Treue and Martínez Trujillo, 1999), spatial (McAdams and Maunsell, 1999), and a third type called object-based attention (Roelfsema et al., 1998). One controversial topic in attentional research has been whether the two former types of attention share similar neural mechanisms. In this issue of Neuron, two different electrophysiological studies using advanced Adriamycin price methodologies in behaving monkeys yield novel, complementary insights into this topic. In the first study,

Zhou and Desimone (2011) conducted simultaneous recordings from areas V4 and the frontal eye fields (FEF) of macaque monkeys during a visual

search task that selleckchem required the animals to memorize a visual cue presented at the beginning of a trial and then search, in a display composed of an array of different objects, for the one that matches the cue by directing gaze to single items (Figure 1A). Area V4 is located at a relatively early stage in the visual processing pathways and contains neurons selective for the color and shape of visual stimuli (Desimone and Schein, 1987). The FEF is located in the prefrontal cortex and contains neurons that encode the position of a visual stimulus, as well as the intended gaze position (Tehovnik et al., 2000). Some degree of shape selectivity has been reported in FEF neurons (Peng et al., 2008). Over the last decade, some studies have supported the role of the FEF as a source of top-down spatial attention signals that reach neurons in area V4 and modulate their sensitivity to visual inputs (Gregoriou et al., 2009 and Moore

and Armstrong, 2003). So far, the FEF role in feature-based attention has remained unclear. Zhou and Desimone (2011) found that during the visual search task, neurons in V4 and the FEF respond more strongly to the target stimulus or Methisazone to stimuli sharing the target features than to other stimuli. The authors discarded the possible role of spatial attention by analyzing trials in which saccades were made to a stimulus away from the receptive fields of the recorded neurons. Because in these trials the focus of spatial attention was not on the stimulus inside the neurons’ receptive fields but instead elsewhere at the position of the future saccadic eye movement, the authors conclude that the increase in response to stimuli matching the attended features was due to feature-based attention. Essential to their findings was that (1) the latency of this effect was shorter in FEF than in V4 neurons, and (2) the intensity of the response modulation was predictive of the efficiency of the visual search—as quantified by the number of saccades needed to find the target. This demonstrates that the FEF is a potential source of top-down signals during tasks that require feature-based attention.

However, there are the clusters of ApNRX and ApNLG that do not co

However, there are the clusters of ApNRX and ApNLG that do not colocalize especially at the distal neuritis, which may represent, in part, mobile clusters that contribute to preformed scaffolding transport complexes and/or extrasynaptic clusters. Overexpression of neurologin-1 in cultured mammalian neurons increases excitatory postsynaptic currents induced by local extracellular stimulation (Chubykin et al., ABT-263 nmr 2007). Thus, we examined the effect of overexpressing ApNLG in the postsynaptic motor neuron or ApNRX in the presynaptic sensory neuron on the strength of

the sensory-to-motor neuron synaptic connection. Overexpression of ApNRX alone in the presynaptic sensory neuron or ApNLG alone in the postsynaptic motor neuron did not lead to an increase in the amplitude of the evoked excitatory postsynaptic potentials CHIR-99021 research buy (EPSPs) measured at 24 hr

after the injection. However, simultaneous overexpression of ApNRX in the presynaptic sensory neuron and ApNLG in the postsynaptic motor neuron led to a significant increase in the strength of the sensory-to-motor neuron synaptic connection measured at 24 hr after the injection (Figures 3D and 3E; % increase in EPSP amplitude: no expression –6.3 ± 4.2, n = 27; ApNRX expression alone –15.6 ± 8.0, n = 6; ApNLG expression alone −6.7 ± 6.1, n = 8; ApNRX and ApNLG expression 61.1 ± 27.5, n = 10, p < 0.001 versus no expression). Thus, the concomitant overexpression of ApNRX in the about presynaptic sensory neuron and ApNLG in the postsynaptic motor neuron can, by itself in the absence of 5-HT training, induce long-lasting synaptic facilitation.

These results support the idea of a functional transsynaptic interaction between ApNRX and ApNLG since ApNRX and ApNLG bind to each other and the overexpression of either ApNRX or ApNLG alone does not induce long-lasting synaptic facilitation. When the whole-cell marker Alexa-594 was injected into sensory neurons in combination with presynaptic overexpression of the ApNRX-GFP construct, it became evident that some presynaptic sensory neuron varicosities are completely filled with ApNRX whereas other varicosities are only partially filled and some varicosities appear to lack ApNRX entirely (Figure 4A). This heterogeneous distribution is similar to the pattern reported for other presynaptic markers in Aplysia such as synaptophysin ( Kim et al., 2003) and allowed us to examine, by time-lapse imaging of living cells in culture, the time course and spatial distribution of ApNRX that may be recruited to the individual presynaptic sensory neuron varicosities during the development of LTF.

Subjects reported the note configurations from left to right The

Subjects reported the note configurations from left to right. The top line mapped onto the leftmost key using the leftmost finger and the bottom line was mapped onto the rightmost key using the rightmost finger. Each 12-element sequence contained 3 notes per line. The notes were randomly ordered without repetition and were free of regularities such as runs (123) and trills (121) with the exception of

one frequently trained sequence (see below) that contained a trill. www.selleckchem.com/products/MLN8237.html The number and order of sequence trials were identical for all subjects, with the exception of two who each missed one run of training due to technical difficulties. A trial began with a fixation signal, which was displayed for 2 s. The complete sequence was presented immediately afterward, and subjects responded as quickly as

possible. They had 8 s to type each sequence correctly. The sequence was present for the entire duration that subjects typed. If a sequence was reported correctly, selleck compound the notes were replaced with a fixation signal until the trial duration was reached. If a participant responded incorrectly, the verbal cue “INCORRECT” appeared and the participant waited for the next trial. Trials not finished within the time limit were counted as incorrect. Subjects trained on 16 different sequences at three different levels of training exposure. Three sequences were trained frequently; with 189 trials for each sequence, and uniformly distributed across the training sessions. These “frequent sequences” are the focus of the present manuscript. The following frequent sequences were presented: s1, 324124134132; s2, 342142134312; and s3, 231431241342. These numbers

indicate the placement of the musical note on the staff: notes on the top line are represented by a 1 while Carboplatin notes on the bottom line are represented by a 4. In addition, there was a second set of three sequences, each presented for 30 trials, and a third set of ten sequences, each presented for between four and eight trials, during training. For the remainder of this paper, we report the results for the three frequent sequences. Frequent sequences were practiced in blocks of 10 trials, with 9 out of 10 being the same frequent sequence, and the other a rare sequence. Trials were separated by an interstimulus interval between 0 s and 20 s, not including time remaining from the previous trial. Following the completion of each block, and in order to motivate subjects, feedback was presented that detailed the number of correct trials and the mean time needed to complete a sequence for the block. Training epochs contained 40 trials (i.e., four blocks) and lasted 345 scans. Each training session contained six scan epochs and lasted a total of 2,070 scans. Stimulus presentation was controlled with a laptop computer running MATLAB 7.1 (Mathworks, Natick, MA) in conjunction with Cogent 2000.

These experiments suggest that proboscis extension is triggered b

These experiments suggest that proboscis extension is triggered by dopamine release from TH-Gal4 neurons acting on D2R, but not DopR. To examine when dopamine is likely

to regulate proboscis extension, we stimulated flies with altered dopaminergic activity with a range of sugar concentrations under different starvation conditions. Flies in which TH-Gal4 neurons were silenced by conditional expression of UAS-Kir2.1 in adults showed decreased probability of extension, as expected (Figures 1C and 3A). As starvation time increased, the response increased, arguing that these flies are still sensitive to other cues related to internal state. However, the response was blunted check details Ion Channel Ligand Library for the highest sugar concentrations, indicating that loss of dopaminergic activity decreases the gain of the response. In the converse experiment, the electrical excitability of dopaminergic neurons was increased by conditional expression of UAS-NaChBac, a low-threshold, slowly inactivating sodium channel. Unlike dTRPA1, this

channel does not drive neural activity by exogenous cues, but instead amplifies the cellular response to membrane depolarization ( Nitabach et al., 2006). Expression of NaChBac in the adult increased the probability of response for all concentrations and starvation conditions ( Figure 3B). Flies with altered dopaminergic activity did not differ in proboscis extension responses to denatonium, a bitter compound, or water, a nonnutritive but acceptable substance (see Figure S1 available online). This result

argues that dopaminergic activity selectively alters the probability of proboscis extension to sucrose, but not to nonnutritious compounds. The probability of proboscis extension depends on sucrose concentration and satiety state. Previous studies have shown that the PR-171 mw activity of gustatory sensory neurons dramatically increases with sucrose concentration (Hiroi et al., 2002 and Marella et al., 2006). The concentration-dependent change in PER probability most likely reflects changes in sensory activity propagating through the circuit. The satiety state also acts to adjust probability of extension, with increased extension to a given concentration occurring when the fly is food deprived. Our behavioral studies argue that the activity of TH-Gal4 neurons serves to adjust the probability of extension to a given sucrose concentration. Thus, dopaminergic neural activity acts as a gain control mechanism to adjust the dynamic range for proboscis extension to sucrose, increasing extension probability when activity is high and decreasing it when it is low.

The essential role for larval ORNs in PN dendrite targeting is ev

The essential role for larval ORNs in PN dendrite targeting is evident from the significant difference between the dendrite targeting

defects at the two temperatures. To test whether Sema-2a derived from larval ORNs is necessary for dendrite targeting of dorsolateral-targeting PNs, we next asked whether RNAi knockdown of Sema-2a in ORNs affected PN dendrite position. Because Sema-2a and Sema-2b function redundantly (Figure 3), see more sema-2a loss-of-function alone should not cause PN dendrite mistargeting. We thus performed Sema-2a RNAi knockdown in sema-2b−/− mutant animals using the ORN-specific pebbled-GAL4 driver. We additionally included one mutant copy of sema-2a to reduce the amount of Sema-2a and sensitize the animals to RNAi knockdown. Flies heterozygous for sema-2a and sema-2b exhibited no dendrite targeting defects ( Figures 6A and 6D, compared to Figure 3J). Flies homozygous mutant for sema-2b and heterozygous for sema-2a exhibited a small but significant ventromedial shift of Mz19+ PN dendrite targeting ( Figures 6B and 6D).

However, when Sema-2a was additionally knocked down in ORNs, we found an additional significant ventromedial shift for Mz19+ PN dendrites ( Figures 6C and 6D). From this experiment alone, we cannot distinguish whether the ventromedial shift of Mz19+ dendrites is caused by Sema-2a function in RO4929097 larval ORNs, adult ORNs, or both, as both populations express pebbled-GAL4. However, several lines of evidence suggest that larval ORNs make a major contribution. First, larval ORNs contributed significantly to the Sema-2a protein distribution pattern in the ventromedial antennal lobe prior to arrival of adult ORN axons ( Figures 4D and 4E). Second, adult PN dendrite patterning occurs before arrival of adult ORN axons. Third, ablating larval ORNs caused

a ventromedial shift in dendrite targeting, just as in sema-2a sema-2b Protein kinase N1 double mutants. Taken together, these experiments strongly suggest that Sema-2a contributed by larval ORNs repels dorsolateral-targeting PNs from the ventromedial antennal lobe. To confirm that larval ORN-derived Sema-2a restricts PN targeting to the dorsolateral antennal lobe, we tested whether Sema-2a overexpression in ORNs was sufficient to rescue the mistargeting of normally dorsolateral-targeting PNs. In sema-2a−/− sema-2b−/− mutant flies, Sema-2a overexpression with pebbled-GAL4 was sufficient to rescue the ventromedial targeting defects of Mz19+ PN dendrites ( Figures 6E–6H), supporting the notion that Sema-2a from larval ORNs plays an essential role in regulating dendrite targeting of adult PNs.

To capture that, we devised a formal method to assign weights to

To capture that, we devised a formal method to assign weights to individual genes reflecting their contribution to high scoring clusters. The method is based on two distributions over clusters: p(C), in which clusters with high scores are assigned a high probability, and a uniform distribution, pu(C), in which all clusters

are equally likely (See Supplemental Experimental Procedures). Each individual gene was then given a score equal to the ratio of the number of clusters that contain the gene sampled from p(C) to the number sampled from pu(C). As a result, the genes which were more frequently included in high-scoring clusters were assigned higher ratios. We used Markov-Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) to sample 5 million clusters from each of the two distributions. To characterize the identified cluster we investigated its interactions with a collection of a priori defined click here functional sets of human genes. For this purpose, we utilized the 1454 gene sets corresponding to the gene ontology (GO) categories used in the MSigDB

database (Subramanian JNJ-26481585 ic50 et al., 2005). Using the background likelihood network, we calculated, for each gene set, its average interaction to the identified cluster shown in Figure 2. To determine the significance of the calculated interaction scores we built gene set-specific background distributions by generating random clusters from the randomized genomic regions with the same gene count as in Levy et al. (2011). We used the background distribution to assign an empirical p-value for every gene set, and then applied the FDR procedure to address the multiple hypotheses involved in testing all gene sets within the collection (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures). This work was supported in part by a grant from the Simons Foundation (SFARI award number SF51

to M.W.), the National Centers for Biomedical Computing (MAGNet) grant U54CA121852 to Columbia University. S.R.G. was buy Dolutegravir supported by the training grant T32 GM082797. We are grateful to all of the families at the participating SFARI Simplex Collection (SSC) sites, as well as the principal investigators (A. Beaudet, R. Bernier, J. Constantino, E. Cook, E. Fombonne, D. Geschwind, D. Grice, A. Klin, R. Kochel, D. Ledbetter, C. Lord, C. Martin, D. Martin, R. Maxim, J. Miles, O. Ousley, B. Pelphrey, B. Peterson, J. Piggot, C. Saulnier, M. State, W. Stone, J. Sutcliffe, C. Walsh, E. Wijsman). We would also like to sincerely thank Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative for generous financial support, Linda Van Aelst, Thomas Jessell, Gerald Fischbach, Marian Carlson, Alan Packer, Barry Honig, Itsik Pe’er, Lauren DeMaria, and Stephen Johnson for helpful discussions. “
“In the adult hippocampus, the process of neurogenesis (the birth, differentiation, and survival of neurons) is highly susceptible to experimental manipulation of external and internal milieus.

A two-tailed unpaired Student’s t test was used for presynaptic a

A two-tailed unpaired Student’s t test was used for presynaptic arbor size analysis and B-Raf inhibitor drug western blot analysis unless otherwise noted. The Mann-Whitney test was used for real-time PCR experiments. p values smaller than 0.05 were considered statistically significant. All p values are indicated as *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001. Data are

presented as mean ± SEM. We thank Dr. Tzumin Lee, Dr. Larry Zipursky, Dr. Catherine Collins, Dr. Chunlai Wu, Dr. Kendal Broadie, Dr. Chun Han, Dr. Liqun Luo, and Dr. Yuh Nung Jan for generously sharing reagents, Dr. Ting Han and Dr. John Kim for their help on the RNA-IP experiments, the members of Dr. Jiandie Lin’s laboratory for helping us to set up the real-time PCR experiments. We also thank Dr. Catherine Collins, Dr. Tzumin Lee, Dr. Hisashi Umemori, and Gabriella Sterne for critical comments on earlier versions of the manuscript. This work was supported by grants from NIH (R00MH080599 and R01MH091186), the Whitehall Foundation, and the Pew Scholars Program in the Biological Sciences to B.Y. “
“The basal ganglia comprise a group of subcortical Autophagy animal study nuclei that includes the striatum, the globus

pallidus, and the substantia nigra. These nuclei receive input from the cerebral cortex and send output to the thalamus, constituting corticobasal ganglia-thalamocortical loops that govern Mirabegron various brain functions associated with complex motor action, reward-based learning, cognition, emotion, and motivation (Redgrave et al., 2010; Utter and Basso, 2008). To perform these different functions, individual cortical areas project to discrete regions of the basal ganglia in a highly topographic manner (Alexander and Crutcher, 1990; Redgrave et al.,

2010). Thus, prefrontal cortical areas provide input to anterior regions of the striatum; sensorimotor cortical areas project to central dorsolateral regions; and the parietal cortex provides input to more posterior regions (Draganski et al., 2008; Takada et al., 2001; Wiesendanger et al., 2004). Dysfunctions along the corticobasal ganglia circuit lead to neurological and neuropsychiatric diseases, including Parkinson’s disease, obsessive-compulsive disorder, schizophrenia, and depressive disorder (Krishnan and Nestler, 2008; Simpson et al., 2010; Utter and Basso, 2008). Therefore, clarification of the precise topography and pathway-specific synapse development in corticobasal ganglia circuits is crucial for understanding the mechanisms that regulate respective brain functions. The anatomical topography of neural circuits generally emphasizes distinct functional units. Functional establishment of this topography requires circuit-specific differentiation and refinement of synapses.

, 2002) The in vivo significance of NALCN’s EEKE motif has been

, 2002). The in vivo significance of NALCN’s EEKE motif has been demonstrated by the finding that a mutant cDNA encoding an EEEE motif, when transgenically expressed in the Drosophila na mutant,

is much less capable of rescuing the phenotypes than the wild-type cDNA ( Lear Selleck PD98059 et al., 2005). This rescue experiment with pore mutants also provided the in vivo evidence confirming that NALCN is indeed an ion channel. Currently, the only available high-resolution structure in the NALCN/CaV/NaV/ CatSper/NaVBac superfamily is that of a bacterial voltage-gated Na+ channel isolated from Arcobacter butzleri (NaVAb) ( Payandeh et al., 2011). Given the overall sequence similarity, especially in the pore regions, between NaVAb and other channels in these families, the structure of the NaVAb homotetramer likely has many of the key signatures of NaVs, CaVs, and NALCN. The overall structure of NaVAb is similar to that of the KVs and is composed of an S1-S4

VSD and a channel pore formed by S5-S6. Unique to NaVAb is a large fenestration on the side of the pore. NaVAb also has an additional pore helix (P2) in addition to the helix (P) also found in KV UMI-77 concentration channels. This P2 helix is C-terminal to the P helix and contains the tryptophan residue (W) of the T/SxE/DxW signature found in all the 24-TM channels ( Payandeh et al., 2011; Figure 3B). In addition, C-terminal to the tryptophan residue in the P2 helix are several amino acids that have been shown to influence channel selectivity, as demonstrated for the bacterial voltage-gated Na+ channel NaChBac Evodiamine ( Ren et al., 2001b and Yue et al., 2002). In the homotetrameric NaVAb channel, the four glutamate (E) residues in the T/SxE/DxW

pore signature form the narrowest ring in the pore filter. In the NALCN protein, one of the glutamate residues in repeat III is replaced by a lysine. Many of the Ca2+/Na+ channels consist of multiple subunits. For example, the NaV complex is composed of a pore-forming α subunit and two transmembrane auxiliary subunits, β1 and β2 (Catterall et al., 2002). Similarly, high voltage-gated CaVs contain the pore-forming α1 subunit, an intracellular β subunit, an α2/δ subunit, and, in some cells such as skeletal muscle cells, a transmembrane γ subunit (Catterall, 2011). Likewise, CatSper channels contain four pore-forming subunits (CatSper1–4) and at least three membrane-spanning auxiliary subunits (β, γ, and δ) (Chung et al., 2011 and Ren and Xia, 2010). The subunit composition of the low-voltage gated CaVs (T-type) is not known. Many of the non-pore-forming, auxiliary subunits are essential for various aspects of basic channel function (Arikkath and Campbell, 2003). The elucidation of the NALCN complex has been greatly facilitated by genetic studies in Drosophila, C. elegans, and mice.

The parasite was not detected in heart, muscle or brain homogenat

The parasite was not detected in heart, muscle or brain homogenates from the jaguarundi. The black-eared opossum tissues could not be examined using this assay, because there was no material left. T. gondii was detected in tissues (lung or brains) from positive mice for each of the isolates. Genotyping results of the isolates from the three wild animals at all the

markers are shown in Table 1. Genotyping was also performed at all these markers with all the tested primary samples from the howler monkey and was successful. Three genotypes were detected. The genotypes from the jaguarundi and the black-eared opossum isolates were detected for ABT-199 datasheet the first time in Brazil. The genotype from the red-handed howler monkey isolate has been previously described in an isolate from a goat in Rio Grande do Norte State and in isolates from 10 chickens in seven states of Northeastern Brazil. Most T. gondii isolates genotyped in Brazil are from domestic animals, including free-range chickens, cats, dogs, sheep and goat; little is known about the genetics of T. gondii isolates from wild mammals in Brazil. Yai et al. (2009) genotyped isolates from capybaras (H. hydrochaeris), the largest rodent in the world, widely present in tropical America; among the 16 genotypes identified from the 36 studied isolates, seven genotypes, corresponding to 10 isolates, were described for the first time and eight of the isolates were grouped into

the common clonal lineages in Brazil, designated as Types BrI, BrII and BrIII ( Pena et al., 2008). In the present study, we isolated ZVADFMK and genotyped T. gondii from three different species of wild mammals in Brazil. These animals were chosen because of convenience. The red-handed howler monkey (A. belzebul) and the jaguarundi (P. yagouaroundi)

were captive animals, inhabiting the same zoo in a state of Northeastern Brazil. Many species of wild animals in Brazil are kept in zoos or by animal breeders as part of conservation programs. Serological studies showed a high prevalence of anti-T. gondii antibodies in zoo animals ( Silva et al., SDHB 2001 and Spencer et al., 2003). Brazil is the richest country in the world in terms of primate species. Red-handed howler monkeys, fed on leaves, fruits and insects, are endemic to Brazil and inhabit the northern and northeastern regions. Currently, there are no reports regarding the seroprevalence of T. gondii antibodies in this species. Garcia et al. (2005) observed a seroprevalence of 17.6% (3/17) in captured wild Alouatta caraya (black and golden howler monkeys) in the southern region. In the present study, we isolated T. gondii from a red-handed howler monkey. It is the first isolation of T. gondii in this species. This animal was suspected of dying of toxoplasmosis. Neotropical primates are one of the most susceptible groups to clinical and fatal toxoplasmosis ( Dubey and Beattie, 1988 and Garrel, 1999).

To increase basal rate of NLG1 cleavage, cultures were incubated

To increase basal rate of NLG1 cleavage, cultures were incubated with bicuculline (50 μM) and 4AP (25 μM) 2 days prior to imaging (Figures 3A and 3B). Interestingly, terminals apposing synapses with GFP-NLG1-ΔSD3 exhibited faster FM4-64 unloading kinetics (τ = 46.1 ± 1.2 s; Figure 6G) than terminals contacting GFP-NLG1-expressing cells (τ = 60.5 ± 1.5 s), indicating that blocking NLG1 cleavage increases presynaptic

release probability. To address whether cleavage of NLG1 is regulated by activity in vivo, we measured NLG1-NTFs generated during MK 1775 pilocarpine-induced status epilepticus (PSE) in mice. Intraperitoneal administration of pilocarpine in P60 mice induced robust seizures and resulted in a 2.2 ± 0.3-fold increase of soluble NLG1-NTFs in the hippocampus after 2 hr PSE (Figures 7A–7C). To test whether MMP9 is involved in PSE-induced NLG1 cleavage, we performed pilocarpine injections in MMP9 KO mice. Notably, 2 hr

PSE characterized by robust behavioral seizures failed to elevate soluble NLG1-NTFs in MMP9 KO hippocampus (1.1 ± 0.1 relatively to control; Selleckchem FRAX597 Figures 7B and 7C). As a control for epileptic activity, both WT and MMP9 KO mice exhibited upregulation of the activity-regulated protein Arc/Arg3.1 after PSE (Figure 7B). Given the enrichment of NLG1-NTFs during the first postnatal weeks (Figures 2G and 2H), we addressed whether NLG1 cleavage is regulated by sensory experience during development. For this, we subjected mice to 5 days of dark rearing (DR) from P21–P26, a period of heightened sensory-evoked refinement of visual cortical circuits (Hensch, 2004), and subsequently re-exposed them to light for a brief period of 2 hr (DR+2hL, Figure 7D). This protocol induces rapid synaptic remodeling in the visual cortex and results alsactide in extensive molecular, functional, and structural synaptic changes (Philpot et al., 2001; Tropea et al., 2010). With this paradigm, 2 hr of re-exposure to light after 5 days of DR caused an increase in NLG1

cleavage in the visual cortex of WT mice (DR: 1.0 ± 0.1; DR+2hL: 1.5 ± 0.2, relatively to light-reared (LR) group; Figures 7E and 7F), but not in MMP9 KO animals (DR: 0.9 ± 0.1; DR+2hL: 1.0 ± 0.1; relative to LR group). Together these findings indicate that increased neuronal activity in vivo triggers MMP9-dependent cleavage of NLG1 in both mature and developing circuits. Although implicated in diverse forms of activity-dependent synaptic maturation and plasticity (Choi et al., 2011; Chubykin et al., 2007; Jung et al., 2010), it has been unclear whether neuroligins acutely regulate synapse function and whether the neuroligin-neurexin transsynaptic complex undergoes dynamic dissociation. Here we have shown that increased neuronal activity decreases synaptic NLG1 in minutes.