, 2009) The cell cycle dynamics of NG2-glia is known from cumula

, 2009). The cell cycle dynamics of NG2-glia is known from cumulative BrdU labeling experiments (Psachoulia et al., 2009 and Simon et al.,

2011; see below). The scale of adult oligogenesis has been something of a surprise. Rivers et al. (2008) calculated that ∼29% of all differentiated oligodendrocytes (identified by CC1 immunolabeling) that are present in the corpus callosum of ∼8-month-old mice are generated in the 210 days after P45 (Pdgfra-CreER∗: Rosa26-YFP). Zhu et al. (2011) found that ∼30% of CC1-positive oligodendrocytes in the corpus callosum OSI-906 research buy of ∼4-month-old mice were formed in the 60 days after P60 (NG2-CreER∗: Rosa26-YFP). Comparing these estimates, one might conclude that no more oligodendrocytes are formed after 4 months of age, but Psachoulia et al. (2009) showed clearly that new myelinating oligodendrocytes are still being formed at a low rate even at 8 months. There are a lot of MEK activation uncertainties in such calculations, (e.g., potential variation in recombination efficiencies from experiment to experiment and at different ages) but, nevertheless, it is clear that oligodendrocyte differentiation continues well

into adulthood ( Figure 1E), though at a steadily decreasing rate ( Rivers et al., 2008, Lasiene et al., 2009, Psachoulia et al., 2009, Kang et al., 2010, Simon et al., 2011 and Zhu et al., 2011). NG2-glia in the cortical gray matter also continue to generate oligodendrocytes into adulthood, although the overall rate of oligogenesis in the cortex is considerably no less than in the corpus callosum at most ages ( Rivers et al., 2008,

Kang et al., 2010, Simon et al., 2011 and Zhu et al., 2011). It is not known yet whether adult myelin genesis is required to replace myelin that degenerates through normal “wear and tear” or whether it adds to existing myelin. Only around 30% of axons in the corpus callosum of 8-month-old mice are fully myelinated (Sturrock, 1980), so there is plenty of scope there and in other major white matter tracts for de novo myelination of previously naked axons. There is evidence from cumulative [3H]-thymidine labeling that oligodendrocytes accumulate modestly in the mouse corpus callosum during the first year, without significant turnover, supporting the idea of de novo myelination (McCarthy and Leblond, 1988). Electron microscopy also shows that the number of myelinated axons in the rodent corpus callosum increases well into adulthood (Nuñez et al., 2000 and Yates and Juraska, 2007).

, 2009) Given the wide expression of Nalcn in the nervous system

, 2009). Given the wide expression of Nalcn in the nervous system, it is not surprising that NALCN is involved in a diffuse array of additional functions. Both worm and fly Nalcn mutants have an altered sensitivity to anesthetics such as halothane ( Humphrey et al., 2007 and Nash et al., 2002). It’s not clear whether the NALCN complex is a direct target of the drugs, or if the altered sensitivity is a result of a disruption in the balance between the hyperpolarizing

activity of K+ channels and the depolarization provided by NALCN. Finally, heterozygous Unc79 mutant mice display a hypersensitivity to alcohol and an increased voluntary alcohol consumption ( Speca et al., 2010). SP-regulated, INALCN-like current Selleck BI6727 Selleck BMS 387032 has been detected in VTA dopaminergic neurons and spinal cord sensory neurons, implicating NALCN in a wide array of animal responses such as pain sensation and reward-seeking behavior. The future use of

conditional knockout mice carrying Nalcn deletion in specific brain and spinal cord regions during selected time windows should be useful in determining whether regulation of NALCN is important for important animal behaviors such as pain sensation and substance addiction. As is the case for K+ leak conductances, Na+ leak currents have been known by physiologists for over 60 years. In recent years, researchers have finally identified a Na+ leak channel, elucidated the members of the channel complex, and revealed some of this channel’s fundamental roles in neuronal function

and animal behavior. Surprisingly, the NALCN channel is not completely selective, but instead is permeable to both Na+ and K+. The basal PNa/PK in neurons is very low (4% in the squid giant axon [Hodgkin and Katz, 1949b]). Under certain conditions, as a result of the synergistic actions between the G protein-dependent and -independent GPCR signaling pathways, the NALCN current can be increased more than 20-fold over basal levels. Given the dominant contribution of NALCN to basal PNa, this synergistic action may provide a powerful non-inactivating excitation to the neurons. Future studies using NALCN blockers and conditional knockout mice will reveal the specific excitatory actions of NALCN in different types of neurons, as well as in the various compartment unless of a single neuron. UNC79 and UNC80 are large and highly-conserved proteins that, as yet, have no identifiable domains. It is perhaps not likely that these two large proteins function solely in channel conduction per se. Future studies may reveal “nonchannel” functions of these proteins and their involvement in the regulation of neuronal excitability and plasticity. NALCN-related mutations may also be found to associate with human diseases. In humans, NALCN, UNC79, and UNC80 are encoded by genes that together span a large genomic region of ∼1 Mb on three chromosome locations (13q32, 14q32, and 2q34).

5°, thus ensuring that chattering and twitching were not included

5°, thus ensuring that chattering and twitching were not included (Harvey et al., 2001), (3) the frequency of whisk cycles was between 4 and 20 Hz; and (4) in records based on EMG, the protractor and retractor muscles did not coactivate in phase, as occurs during twitching and chattering (Berg and Kleinfeld, 2003a). Voltage signals from the cortical and EMG microwire electrodes were impedance buffered, amplified, band-pass filtered from 1 Hz to 10 kHz and sampled at 36 kHz (Ganguly and Kleinfeld, 2004).

The cortical recordings were band-pass filtered between 600 Hz and 6 kHz (6 pole Butterworth filter run in forward and reverse directions) to isolate the spectral power of extracellular spike waveforms (Fee et al., 1996b). The voltage difference between the two EMG signals from each implanted muscle was calculated Paclitaxel concentration numerically, band-pass filtered between 400 Hz and 3 kHz (4 pole Butterworth filter run in forward and reverse directions), rectified, low-pass filtered at 250 Hz, and down-sampled to 1 kHz to form the differential rectified EMG signal (|∇EMG|). Cortical recordings were analyzed with an offline non-Gaussian cluster analysis algorithm to obtain check details single unit spike trains (Fee et al., 1996a). Putative

single units were accepted for analysis if the number of spikes that violated an imposed absolute refractory period of 2.5 ms was consistent with less than 10% level of contamination by unresolved units with Poisson spike rates. Further, the waveforms of the putative single units were visually inspected for separation from background noise and other waveform clusters obtained in the same recordings. We estimated

false-negative and false-positive errors (Hill et al., 2011) and found that 75% of our putative single unit clusters had a false-negative contamination of less than 10%, while 90% contained less than 20% contamination. In addition, that 88% of our putative single unit clusters had a false-positive contamination of less than 10%, while 95% contained less than 20% contamination. The relatively small false-positive before rate supports the claim that the same single units can code multiple stimulus dimensions (Figure 4, Figure 5 and Figure 7). These particular quality metrics could not be applied to the reevaluation of the data set from vS1 cortex (Figure S5). The correlation between a set of signals may be defined through the singular value decomposition (Golub and Kahan, 1965), a standard matrix factorization procedure that has previously been applied to determine correlations within space-time data (Prechtl et al., 1997). For the case of whisking motion across multiple vibrissae, we define the matrix Θ(x,t), where x labels the individual vibrissa and t is discrete time.

For instance, the neuronal early endosome protein NEEP21 (origina

For instance, the neuronal early endosome protein NEEP21 (originally identified as Neural Specific Gene 1, Nsg1; Sabéran-Djoneidi et al., 1998 and Sutcliffe Selleck OSI-906 et al., 1983) is expressed primarily in neurons and is found in an early endosomal population largely distinct from

EEA1-positive endosomes (Steiner et al., 2002). NEEP21 interacts with the SNARE protein syntaxin13 and localizes to rab4-positive but rab5-negative domains of early endosomes (Steiner et al., 2002). NEEP21-positive endosomes accumulate endocytosed L1/NgCAM adhesion molecules, as well as AMPA receptors (Steiner et al., 2005, Steiner et al., 2002 and Yap et al., 2008) and are involved in trafficking of multiple cargos (Alberi et al., 2005, Debaigt et al., 2004, Steiner et al., 2005, Steiner et al., 2002 and Yap et al., 2008). NEEP21 also binds to GRIP1, an interaction important for GluR2 trafficking (Steiner et al., 2005). Recently, NEEP21 was shown to interact with and affect proteolytic processing of βAPP Raf inhibitor review (Norstrom et al., 2010). The precise mode of NEEP21 action, the role of its interaction with syntaxin13, and what neuronal-specific role it might play are still unknown. Another neuronal-specific protein is GRASP-1, which is an effector of Rab4 and an important component of the molecular machinery that coordinates RE maturation in dendrites. GRASP-1 is also necessary for

AMPAR recycling, maintenance of spine morphology, and synaptic plasticity (Hoogenraad et al., 2010). It will be important to elucidate how these neuronal-specific components modify the canonical machinery to achieve neuron-specific functions. Some canonical endosomal regulators have specialized functions in neurons. For example, in nonneuronal cells members of the EHD family, EHD1-EHD4, regulate trafficking through early and recycling endosomes (Grant and Caplan, 2008). EHD1 associates Megestrol Acetate with pre-existing tubules in fibroblasts (Sharma et al., 2009), but in neurons tubular EHD1-containing

compartments are virtually absent. Rather, EHD1 colocalizes predominantly with round EEA1-positive EEs. Live imaging showed that EHD1 precedes EEA1 on EEs and often persists even after EEA1 has dissociated (Yap et al., 2010). Interestingly, in neurons EHD4 (also called pincher) and EHD1 are involved in endocytosis (Shao et al., 2002), rather than (or in addition to) recycling (Sharma et al., 2008). For instance, Nogo-A, a repulsive cue for axon growth cones, was shown to be endocytosed by an EHD4/pincher pathway (Joset et al., 2010). L1/NgCAM uses an EHD1/EHD4-dependent pathway for endocytosis. This pathway is cargo specific and cell type specific (Yap et al., 2010). EHD4 (possibly as a heterodimer with EHD1) thus mediates a specialized internalization pathway in neurons. Since EHD proteins interact with multiple trafficking regulators via their C-terminal EH domains (Naslavsky and Caplan, 2011), they regulate and coordinate recruitment and activation of other effectors classes, such as rabs (Jovic et al., 2010).

After differentiation, Notch signaling inhibits neurite extension

After differentiation, Notch signaling inhibits neurite extension in cultured vertebrate neurons and in the

neonatal mouse cortex (Berezovska et al., 1999, Franklin et al., 1999, Redmond et al., 2000 and Sestan et al., 1999) and modulates axon guidance in Drosophila ( Crowner et al., 2003). Our results demonstrate that Notch’s function in regulating the growth potential of neurons is not limited to development. Rather, Notch signaling can function long after development is complete and can act after nerve injury to suppress axon regeneration. Animals were maintained on nematode growth medium agar plates with E. coli OP50 as a source of food ( Stiernagle, 2006). Temperature was controlled at 20°C unless otherwise stated. Null mutations in lin-12 result in sterility, 3-Methyladenine cost so we characterized homozygous mutant progeny that segregated from a balanced heterozygous Afatinib order strain. Maternal contributions of wild-type Notch/lin-12 allow these mutants to survive and develop into viable adults. Many of these adults rupture from their vulva; we used only normally sized, healthy animals in these experiments. Strain names, genotypes, and complete data with p values can be found in Tables S1–S3. All experiments were performed in parallel with a matched control. L4-stage hermaphrodites were mounted in a slurry of 0.1 μm diameter polystyrene beads (Polysciences) or in 50 mM of the GABA

SB-3CT agonist, muscimol, (Sigma M1523) to immobilize the animals. No difference in regeneration rates was observed between beads and muscimol: wild-type animals regenerated at a similar rate under both conditions, and Notch signaling mutants had increased regeneration under both conditions (data not shown). Commissures in the tail region of the animal posterior to the vulva were severed (GABA neurons: VD and DD; acetylcholine neurons: AS and DB). Commissures were visualized with a Nikon Eclipse 80i microscope using a 100× Plan ApoVC lens (1.4 NA) and a Hamamatsu Orca camera. Selected axons were cut

using a Micropoint laser from Photonic Instruments (10 pulses, 20 Hz). Axotomized animals were recovered to agar plates and remounted 18–24 hr later for scoring. At least 30 axons were scored for most genotypes (2–3 cut axons per animal); see Tables S1–S3. Only axons with a distal stump as evidence of a complete cut were scored. Axons with a visible growth cone that had progressed past the cut site, and axons that had regenerated to the dorsal nerve cord, were scored as positive. Axons with no growth or with only filopodial extensions and no progression past the cut site were counted as negative. When scoring full regeneration, only axons that showed visual evidence of reconnection to the dorsal cord 24 hr after axotomy were scored as positive. For growth cone initiation at 4 and 6 hr, axons with a growth cone were scored as positive.

We thank Matt Kleinman for his help in collecting

We thank Matt Kleinman for his help in collecting Ion Channel Ligand Library mw the data and Drs. Anouk Scheres, James Rilling, and Lynn Nadel for their helpful comments. We would like to acknowledge funding from the National Institute of Aging (R21AG030768) to A.G.S., the

National Institute of Mental Health (R03MH077058) to A.G.S. and (F31MH085465) to L.J.C., and the National Science Foundation to M.D. “
“In this issue of Neuron, we present a series of Reviews and Perspectives on neural stem cells and neurogenesis. The study of neural stem cells (NSCs) has provided fundamental insight into how the exquisite diversity of neurons and glia in the nervous system is achieved. And the role of NSCs does not end with development. While it was once thought that the production of neurons ends early in life and the adult brain has little potential for regeneration, the discovery of adult neurogenesis radically changed this

view. We now know that neurogenesis occurs throughout life, giving us hope that this regenerative potential might be harnessed for the development of therapies for neurological Nutlin-3a price disorders. Given that this is such an active area of research, it wasn’t possible to cover the full spectrum of the field in a single issue. Our goal was to highlight a range of topics and some of the conceptual themes that have emerged from recent work. These Reviews and Perspectives discuss how the study of NSC biology and adult neurogenesis has provided fundamental insight into brain development and function. The potential clinical implications of this work are also considered. One of the central debates in the field is the focus of a pair of Point/Counterpoint pieces by Rene Hen and colleagues and Fred Gage and colleagues. In these pieces, the authors discuss their distinct views of the role of adult born neurons in cognition. In either addition, this collection also contains two NeuroViews that look at stem cell research from a global and societal perspective. In his piece

Douglas Sipp, from the RIKEN Center for Developmental Biology, discusses how stem cell research is a global enterprise, commenting on the resources and challenges for stem cell researchers across the globe. In the second NeuroView Patrick Taylor, Chair of the ISSCR Task Force on Unproven Stem Cell Treatments, considers how active engagement between scientists and the public, as well as ethicists and the government, has transformed the landscape of stem cell research. In their May issue, our colleagues at Cell Stem Cell published a special collection of Reviews and research papers on NSCs. The Neuron and Cell Stem Cell articles provide a complementary view of the NSC field. We were excited to collaborate with Cell Stem Cell on this project and hope that our goal of highlighting the intersection between the neuroscience and stem cell communities has been realized.

Parent substance use was self-reported usually by the mother and

Parent substance use was self-reported usually by the mother and entails the average number Alectinib cost of alcoholic drinks consumed per week. Age, gender (boy: x = 1; girl: x = 2) and OC use (no: x = 0; yes: x = 1) were assessed using a demographics self-report questionnaire. Height and weight were measured prior to the test session to calculate BMI. Time of test session was coded noon (x = 1) or late afternoon (x = 2). First, a manipulation check was performed by way

of repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) in the entire sample in order to confirm that the stressful tasks did induce an increase in HR and PS as compared to the Rest period. Age and gender were entered into all models as covariates. Prior to the main analysis additional covariates were examined and added to the main analysis if they correlated significantly with both independent and dependent variables. To investigate HR during the stress procedure, a 3 × 3 × 3 RM-ANOVA was performed with period (Rest,

Task, Recovery) as the within-subjects factor and gNDW (Low, Medium, High Quantity) and gFTU (Non-smokers, Low, High Frequency Smokers) as between-subjects factors. Interactions between period and the between-subjects VE 821 variables as well as between period and the covariates were examined. Simple contrasts were performed in order to explore between-group differences. Univariate ANOVAs of the change score in HR between Rest and Task periods were performed when interaction effects were present. An identical analysis was performed with PS as the dependent variable measured across the three periods. In all analyses, Greenhouse–Geisser statistics are reported when necessary to correct departures from sphericity. For the entire sample, the tasks produced physiological stress, as indicated by a significant within-groups effect of period (F(1.32,357.37) = 589.66, p < .001). Simple contrasts showed that average HR during the Task was significantly higher than during Rest (F(1,271) = 412.99, p < .001). PS also differed across the three periods (F(1.51,414.42) = 403.01, p < .001), with simple contrasts again showing PS to be higher

during the Task as compared to Rest (F(1,274) = 293.39, heptaminol p < .001). Descriptives of, and correlations between, dependent, independent and potential covariates are shown in Table 2 and Table 3. Age and gender were controlled in all models. No other variables correlated with both dependent and independent variables, and therefore were not included as covariates in the models. PS and HR showed a small, significant positive correlation, specifically during Task (R = .13, p < .05) and Recovery (R = .12, p < .05), additionally PS Rest with HR Task (R = .13, p < .05) and HR Recovery (R = .16, p < .01). The HR response measure was not significantly correlated with the PS response measure. A significant between-subjects effect was evident for gNDW (F(2,244) = 6.12, p < .01).

carneum, P paneum or P psychrosexualis The latter three specie

carneum, P. paneum or P. psychrosexualis. The latter three species produce several mycotoxins ( Frisvad and Samson, 2004 and Houbraken et al., 2010) and have often been referred to as P. roqueforti ( Engel and von Milczewski, 1977, von Krusch et al., 1977, Olivigni and Bullerman, 1978, Engel and Prokopek, 1980, Teuber and Engel, 1983 and Erdogan and Sert, 2004). However, P. roqueforti itself can produce the secondary metabolites

PR-toxin, roquefortine C, mycophenolic acid and andrastin A in pure culture ( Frisvad et al., 2004 and Nielsen et al., 2005). One of these secondary metabolites is regarded as a mycotoxin, PR-toxin. This mycotoxin is unstable CHIR-99021 price in cheese and is converted to PR-imine ( Engel and Prokopek, 1979 and Siemens and Zawistowski, 1993). Mycophenolic acid ( Lafont et al., 1979 and López-Díaz et al., 1996), roquefortine C ( López-Díaz et al., 1996 and Finoli et al., 2001) and andrastin A ( Nielsen et al., 2005 and Fernández-Bodega et al., 2009) have been found in blue cheese, but the consequences to human health are probably minor ( Larsen et al., 2002). Yet another species, Penicillium solitum is found on naturally fermented lamb meat on the Faroe Islands, and may be used as a starter PFT�� culture. This

species does not produce any known mycotoxins ( Frisvad et al., 2004). On other meat products, Penicillium nalgiovense and few strains of Penicillium chrysogenum are used ( Nout, 2000 and Frisvad and

Samson, 2004), especially for mold-fermented salami. However, P. nalgiovense was originally found on cheeses from Nalzovy, and may be used for fermenting cheeses too. Verticillium lecanii has changed to Lecanicillium lecanii ( Zare and Gams, 2001), and this strain has been listed as potentially Ketanserin useful for cheese ripening (see Table 2 and Table 3). Finally, some fungi can be used to produce food colorants, including Epicoccum nigrum and Penicillium purpurogenum, but these fungi are not used directly for food fermentation ( Stricker et al., 1981 and Mapari et al., 2010). The list of microorganisms with a history of use in food originally included 31 genera in the 2002 IDF inventory, and was essentially limited to the microbial use in dairy matrices. By also considering other food matrices, we consider 62 genera in the 2011 update. One was rejected as its usage in food has not been documented and the initial reference in the 2002 IDF inventory was inadequate. The evolution in taxonomy, the extension of varied usages in other matrices, yeast fermentations and fungal foods have also resulted in a growing number of species; from 113 to 264 species with demonstration of food usage. There are many new possibilities, however, and these should be explored to a much greater extent. Either in traditional fermented foods or as new opportunities, the rationalized use of microorganisms in our diet opens new perspectives.

, 2002) Also, behavioural and psychological symptoms in the elde

, 2002). Also, behavioural and psychological symptoms in the elderly are highly associated with increased burden Navitoclax concentration on carers (Macpherson et al., 1994). The stress of caring for such older adults could be further exacerbated by their drinking behaviour. In this study we also test the specific hypotheses that levels of disability and behavioural symptoms mediate the association between older adults’ heavy drinking behaviour and co-resident psychological morbidity. We chose these two factors as they have been shown to be major mediators of carer burden in dementia and chronic physical illnesses in the elderly (Baumgarten et al., 1992, Deimling and Bass, 1986 and Schrag et al., 2006). The population

of the Dominican Republic is 9.4 million, and 0.5 million (5.7%) are aged 65 and over (Central Intelligence Agency, 2008). This is a comprehensive cross-sectional survey of all residents aged 65 and over living in a geographically defined catchment area in Santo Domingo in the Dominican Republic. Atypical middle-class

or high-income areas were avoided. The catchment areas selected were Villa Francisca, San Carlos, San Antón, Mejoramiento Social and Santa Barbara. After defining the boundaries, Bortezomib mapping was carried out to identify and locate households. This did not include any hospital or nursing home residents. The protocol is described in detail in an open access publication (Prince et al., 2007). The study was approved by Consejo Nacional de Bioetica (CONABIO),

the local ethical committee as well as by the Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College, London. Two thousand older people were interviewed (95% response rate) (Prince et al., 2007). The analysis in this study is limited to 1391 participants Digestive enzyme aged 65 and over who were residing with the informants. Interviewers were instructed to recruit the person who knew the older person best, and could give the clearest and most detailed account of their current circumstances. Co-residents and family members were prioritised unless others were clearly better qualified to give the required information. If there were several co-resident family members, time spent with the older person was the main criterion. Where the older person needed care, then the main caregiver was selected. However, if the main caregiver was paid, the main organisational caregiver was selected instead. Co-residents who were aged 65 years old and over were also participants (n = 371). All assessments were cross-culturally validated and translated into Spanish. Interviews were conducted in participants’ own homes and all participants and co-residents received the assessment in the form of face to face interviews. Information was elicited from participants; with informants also interviewed for those with communication difficulties arising from dementia, severe mental illness, deafness or mutism.

A second question was whether our participants were indeed engagi

A second question was whether our participants were indeed engaging in hierarchical learning and updating their learning rate dynamically, as our Bayesian model assumed, or used a simpler learning mechanism. To clarify this, we added two more models to our comparison set. The models were a Bayesian model

with reduced hierarchical depth (HGF3) in which the third level was eliminated from the hierarchy, and a standard Rescorla-Wagner (RL) model with a fixed learning rate. Finally, we implemented a RL model with click here dynamic learning rate (Sutton, 1992) that was recommended by one of the reviewers as a non-Bayesian alternative to HGF1. See the Supplemental Experimental Procedures section C (available online) for more information on these models. Comparing these five models, we found that, across studies, HGF1 was the superior model in 86 out of our 118 participants. Examining each study separately, random effects BMS yielded posterior model probabilities of 84% (behavioral study), 74% (first fMRI study), and 72% (second fMRI study) for HGF1, which was five to ten times higher than for the next best model in

each case (Table S1). As a consequence, in each study, the exceedance probability in favor of HGF1 (i.e., the probability that its posterior probability was higher than that of any other model considered) (Stephan et al., 2009) was indistinguishable from 100%. These results provide strong evidence that our participants selleck did learn the task-relevant conditional probabilities of visual stimuli (instead of predicting the incidental reward) and were capable of updating their learning rate dynamically. We next examined the estimates of the free parameters (κ, ϑ, ζ) from the winning model (Table S2). These estimates were comparable across the three studies,

as demonstrated by ANOVA: none of the model parameters showed significant differences across studies (κ: F(2,115) = 1.04, p = 0.358; ϑ: F(2,115) = 0.91, p = 0.405; ζ: F(2,115) = 2.98, p = 0.055). Additionally, we used multiple regression to evaluate how well our model explained subjects’ behavior (percentage of correct responses). This quantified model performance Carnitine palmitoyltransferase II in terms of variance explained, complementary to the relative model comparison by BMS above. This analysis showed that the linear combination of the three model parameters predicted subjects’ task performance well (behavioral study: R2 = 0.64, F(3,42) = 25.3, p < 0.001; first fMRI study: R2 = 0.59, F(3,41) = 20.1, p < 0.001; second fMRI study: R2 = 0.63, F(3,23) = 13.2, p < 0.001). As detailed in the Experimental Procedures section, our fMRI analysis focused on precision-weighted PEs and uncertainty estimates across the hierarchical levels of the HGF.