Smaller model subunits of 20 μm diameter,

Smaller model subunits of 20 μm diameter, CP-868596 in vivo which are still larger than typical salamander photoreceptors (Mariani, 1986 and Sherry et al., 1998), are not consistent with the experimental data (Figure 4C), indicating that the nonlinearities do not occur on the level of photoreceptor signals. Although static nonlinear signaling of bipolar cells may underlie the threshold-quadratic

nonlinearity, it cannot explain the striking difference between the shapes of iso-rate and iso-latency curves for homogeneity detectors. To build an intuition for the processes that give rise to this surprising discrepancy, we analyzed the temporal response profiles for different stimuli along the iso-response curves (Figure 5). To do so, we measured iso-response curves and then chose three characteristic points on the curves for repeated measurements of the corresponding stimuli in randomized

fashion. For cells with similar iso-rate and iso-latency curves, we found, as expected, that response patterns had virtually identical temporal structure along iso-rate curves (Figure 5A). For homogeneity detectors, we first consider stimuli that lie along an iso-latency curve (Figure 5B). As a stronger stimulus MK-2206 nmr typically leads to shorter latency (Figure 2D) (Sestokas et al., 1987), the iso-latency condition means that the different stimulus layouts initially were equally effective. Subsequently, however, the activity under stimulation of half the receptive field (Figure 5B, green and orange lines) did not rise as strongly and last as long as for homogeneous stimulation (Figure 5B, black line). Why did the activity not continue in the same way for the two layouts even though the latency suggested them to be equally strong? A plausible interpretation is that spike bursts for stimulation of half the receptive field were affected by a suppression mechanism Thymidine kinase that became effective shortly after the initial phase of the spike burst. This view is consistent with the spike patterns along the iso-rate curves (Figure 5C).

Here, the stimulation of half the receptive field has to occur at considerably higher contrast to enforce the same spike count. During the initial response part, this higher contrast provides a much more potent stimulus, thus leading to shorter response latencies (Sestokas et al., 1987). The response to homogeneous stimulation, on the other hand, starts later and reaches a smaller peak firing rate, corresponding to the much smaller applied contrast. But it compensates by the slightly longer response duration, presumably due to less suppression, to reach the same average spike count. We thus hypothesize that a suppression mechanism acts on homogeneity detectors for strong local stimulation. Note that local stimulation refers to activation of half the receptive field center in our standard stimulus layout, but strong stimulation in smaller regions also triggers the suppression (Figure 3F).

, 2010 and Leutgeb et al , 2007; Figure 1D) Along these lines, w

, 2010 and Leutgeb et al., 2007; Figure 1D). Along these lines, while much of the behavioral literature arguing for a pattern separation function is consistent, there are also alternative explanations. Instead of studying the ability of animals to distinguish Vorinostat solubility dmso different input patterns concurrently,

the behavioral studies of the roles of the DG and neurogenesis in pattern separation have typically been designed to examine how animals’ responses to their present situation can be altered by their memories of the past input patterns (which are different from the current ones). Two types of strategies have been used in behavioral tasks for pattern separation. In some tasks, animals were trained to Selleck GS-7340 distinguish two input patterns, such as conditioned (CS+) and unconditioned (CS−) contexts. Specifically, initial training enabled the animals to generalize their conditioned responses to both CS+ and CS− contexts, and their ability to discriminate the CS+ and CS− contexts was subsequently tested through continuing reinforcement of the CS+ context but not the CS− context (McHugh et al., 2007 and Sahay et al., 2011).

It is possible that performance changes resulting from alterations in DG and/or neurogenesis may be due to defects in pattern separation, but it is also possible that other processes, such as inhibitory learning, may be involved. In other tasks, animals were trained also to learn one pattern and were subsequently tested, using a working memory framework, for their ability to discriminate a learned pattern from another pattern (Clelland et al., 2009, Creer et al., 2010, Gilbert et al., 2001, Hunsaker and Kesner, 2008 and Saxe et al., 2007). Paradigms using this type of task are also able to evaluate behavioral performance as a function

of the extent of input pattern differences such as by varying the distance of spatial location systematically in the cheeseboard spatial discrimination task (Gilbert et al., 2001), further supporting a relationship between the pattern separation ability and behavioral outcome. However, it remains difficult to rule out in these tasks that animals may solve the task using different neural pathways according to the degree of dissimilarity between the input patterns. For example, in the cheeseboard spatial discrimination task, lesions of CA1 did not affect the performance at any of five tested pattern separation degrees, suggesting that the task could be solved independent of the trisynaptic pathway (Gilbert et al., 2001). On the other hand, lesions of CA3 affect working memory in general, making it difficult to test whether pattern separation relies on CA3 outputs other than Schaffer collaterals (Gilbert and Kesner, 2006). Finally, there is a lack of a clear role for young neurons that would make them advantageous in the classic mechanism by which the DG provides separation.

In total, there were 16 legal clauses identified under the three

In total, there were 16 legal clauses identified under the three overarching categories: cost responsibility (5 clauses), sustainability (7 clauses), and scope (4 clauses). Under the scope category, nearly all of the SUAs (n = 17 agreements) included

all of the provisions; one SUA failed to directly address use period. The clauses contained within the other two categories, cost responsibility and sustainability were not as consistently represented. Akt inhibitor Although the clauses on indemnity (in n = 12 agreements), insurance (n = 13), restitution/repairs (n = 12), and liability (n = 13) were included in a majority of the agreements, security was addressed only in less than half of the JUMPP-assisted SUAs (n = 7). Similarly, while clauses in the sustainability category such as state/local law compliance (in n = 18 agreements), communication protocol (n = 11), and operations/maintenance PD-1/PD-L1 inhibitor 2 (n = 13) were included in the majority ( Table 4), other sustainability clauses such as sanitation (n = 9), severability (n = 9), and transferability (n = 7) were only represented in half or less

than half of the agreements ( Table 4). Among the 18 SUAs, the type of agreement appeared to be related to the number and type of clauses that were incorporated as part of each of the three overarching categories. Agreements for Services/Shared-use Agreements and License Agreements contained the highest number of clauses (mean = 15.1 clauses) while Community Recreation Agreements

(mean = 6.7 clauses) and Letter of Agreements (mean = 7.0 clauses) contained the fewest. not In supplemental analysis, the 18 JUMPP-assisted SUAs were estimated to have the potential to reach approximately 29,035 children (ages 5–19) and 89,155 adults (ages 20–64) in the surrounding communities. This estimate was calculated using the census tracts that were included in the 1-mile radius of the school sites and assumed 10% of the population may participate. The estimate represented the potential reach count of people that could potentially participate. Although it has a number of limitations, reach estimates are often used by funding agencies such as the CDC to help plan and make decisions about resource allocations (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2012). Based on a total of $281,515 invested in the JUMPP Task Force effort, it was estimated that approximately 4 community members were reached for every $10 spent during the CPPW-RENEW program ($0.38 per member reached); these cost projections, however, did not account for the programming (if offered) or each school site’s costs of maintaining the opened space/facilities. Many of the concerns noted by the school districts were addressed by the elements found in the SUAs. However legal clauses related to security were surprisingly not as common as expected based on school concerns. This lack of inclusion may affect the continuation of each agreement over time.

Why this strain is free from the early onset behavioral symptoms

Why this strain is free from the early onset behavioral symptoms one would expect in what is essentially an N-terminal transgene HD model is still under investigation. Knockin mouse P450 inhibitor neuropathology, as with their symptoms, usually occurs very late in life and is minor in comparison to transgenic strains. HdhQ72-80 mice demonstrate loss of brain weight by 16 months (Shelbourne et al., 1999), while NIIs are primarily seen in striatal MSNs. Knockins with 94 repeats demonstrate striatal NIIs by 18 months (Menalled et al., 2002), while HdhQ111 mice show NII appearance at

an earlier age (10 months), and mHtt nuclear accumulation is evident at a very early (6 weeks) age (Wheeler et al., 2000). HdhQ150 knockins lose as much as 40% of striatal volume and neurons by 23 months, but significant gliosis and NIIs appear by 10–14 months (Heng et al., 2007, Lin et al., 2001, Tallaksen-Greene et al., 2005 and Yu et al., 2003). Degenerating neurons are not apoptotic in this strain, though occasional

dark neurons are encountered. Knockin mice with 140 CAG repeats display relatively early onset striatal www.selleckchem.com/products/pd-0332991-palbociclib-isethionate.html NIIs and neuropil aggregates (4 months) becoming progressively stronger in other brain regions as well by 6 months (Menalled et al., 2003). These data do not imply that knockin brains only present with abnormality in old age, as HdhQ111 embryos demonstrate impaired neurogenesis as early as embryonic day 13.5 (Molero et al., 2009). Protein context clearly influences neuropathology and symptoms in HD, as other polyglutamine disorders such as SBMA, DRPLA, and the various

spinocerebellar ataxias (reviewed in Yamada et al., 2008) produce distinct vulnerable neuronal populations and motor symptoms. Posttranslational modifications influence progression, as ablation of caspase-6 cleavage sites within the YAC transgene yields a mouse that demonstrates no behavioral symptoms or striatal atrophy along with a delay in mHTT translocation to the nucleus (Graham et al., 2006). Also, while only 17 residues lie N-terminal to the polyQ repeat in human HTT, these Oxygenase play a role in neuropathology; phosphomimetic mutations of serines 13 and 16 prevent aggregation and symptom onset in parallel BACHD lines (Gu et al., 2009). While inclusions are a historic histological hallmark of HD, evidence continues to mount that their presence does not correlate with toxicity, as seen in chimaeric R6/2 or in Ss mice. The theory that inclusions represent a sequestration site for mHTT, while smaller oligomers mediate toxicity, is consistent with neuropathological data in mice but is hard to prove with causative data. It represents an intriguing therapeutic option though, that perhaps altering aggregation kinetics by accelerating macroaggregation or reducing oligomerization would slow toxicity.

Different operations may be advantageous in different contexts (e

Different operations may be advantageous in different contexts (e.g., differentiation for a detection task, or belief propagation for inference about whether two parts of an object are connected). Nonetheless, a simple but powerful Selleckchem BMS 754807 idea is that in many situations evidence is accumulated to some threshold level, whence the decision terminates in a choice, even if provisional (Resulaj et al., 2009). If the two directions are equally likely (i.e., neutral

prior probability), then we represent the process as an accumulation of signal plus noise to symmetric decision bounds (Figure 2A). The upper and lower bounds support termination in favor of a right or left choice, respectively. In the brain, this process looks more like a race between two mechanisms, one that accumulates evidence for right (against left), and the other that does the opposite (Figure 2B). This detail matters for correspondence with the physiology (Figure 3). The beauty of this idea is that a single mechanism can thus account for both which decision is made and how much time (or how many samples) it takes to commit to an answer—in other words, the balance between accuracy and speed. As shown in Figure 3B the framework is so powerful that one can fit the reaction time data to establish the Selleck OSI906 model parameters—an estimate of the bound height and a coefficient that converts motion strength to units

of SNR—and then predict the accuracy at each of the motion strengths (solid curve, upper graph). This is a rare feat in psychophysics: to make

a set of measurements and to use it to predict another. It convinced us that there is merit to the idea (Box 2). Here is a cautionary tale that ought to interest Terminal deoxynucleotidyl transferase theorists, experimentalists, philosophers, and historians of science. The concept of bounded evidence integration originated in the field of quality control, which draws on statistical inference from sequential samples of data. Abraham Wald began this secretly as a way to decide whether batches of munitions were of sufficient quality to ship. He developed the sequential probability ratio test as the optimal procedure to test a hypothesis against its alternative, using the minimal number of samples (effectively a speed versus accuracy tradeoff) (Wald, 1947 and Wald and Wolfowitz, 1947). The test involves accumulation of evidence in the form of a log-likelihood ratio (logLR; or a proportional quantity) to a pair of terminating bounds, which trigger acceptance of the respective hypotheses. Alan Turing developed the same algorithm as a part of his code-breaking work in WWII (Gold and Shadlen, 2002 and Good, 1979). A decade later, several psychologists recognized the implications for choice and reaction time (RT) (e.g., Laming, 1968 and Stone, 1960). However, the field realized that this model predicts that for a fixed stimulus strength (e.g.

Minor allele carriers of the L607F variant in both healthy indivi

Minor allele carriers of the L607F variant in both healthy individual and schizophrenia patients displayed reduced gray matter density in the superior frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate gyrus, and also experienced greater positive symptoms (Szeszko et al., 2008). Interestingly, a recent study from Rapoport and colleagues reported both S704C and L607F variants affect cortical thickness in developing children and adolescents (Raznahan et al., 2010). Selleck I BET151 Here, the authors determined complex interplay between S704C, L607F, and cortical thickness, suggesting these

variants functionally interact to regulate the structure of the developing brain. Together, these studies highlight that common DISC1 variants play an important role in regulating brain structure, function, and neuropsychiatric behavior. However, it is still unclear how these

polymorphisms lead to changes in brain structure and, moreover, whether they disrupt DISC1 signaling pathways that contribute to these effects. Given the different brain structural and functional changes associated with common DISC1 Compound C manufacturer polymorphisms, we hypothesized these effects were due to deleterious effects on brain development via disruption of specific signaling pathways. Various studies have uncovered a number of brain development events and signaling pathways that critically involve DISC1. For example, some of the processes DISC1 regulates includes neurogenesis, neuronal migration, axon/dendrite growth, synaptogenesis, adult neuron generation and synaptic integration, dopaminergic neuron function, and cell-cell adhesion (Enomoto et al., 2009, Hayashi-Takagi et al., 2010, Kamiya et al., 2005, Kamiya et al., 2006, Kim et al., 2009, Lipina et al., 2010b, Mao et al., 2009, Niwa et al., 2010, Pletnikov et al., 2007 and Singh et al., 2010).

This is further complicated by the number of DISC1-dependent pathways that have been identified to date, including signaling via Lis1/Ndel1, phosphodiesterase 4/cyclic adnosine monophosphate (cAMP), interaction with growth neuregulin, amyloid precursor protein (APP), Akt/Girdin signaling, and Rac1, and our lab recently reported that DISC1 modulates canonical Wnt signaling via GSK3β and Dixdc1 (Enomoto et al., 2009, Jaaro-Peled et al., 2009, Kamiya et al., 2006, tuclazepam Kim et al., 2009, Mao et al., 2009, Millar et al., 2005, Shinoda et al., 2007 and Young-Pearse et al., 2010). The finding that DISC1 directly inhibits GSK3β is interesting given that the common mood stabilizer drug lithium, and the schizophrenia risk gene Akt, also inhibit GSK3β which results in activation of canonical Wnt signaling, suggesting it may be an important target in psychiatric disease. Here, we present evidence that human DISC1 variants have a deleterious impact on two brain development events, neurogenesis and neuronal migration, via different signaling pathways.

This in turn makes

This in turn makes LY2157299 cost it possible to identify the factors associated with the selective expansion of certain clones in vivo. We found that the chief determinants of clonal abundance were the transcriptional orientation of the provirus

and its position (upstream or downstream), relative to the nearest host transcriptional start site. Proviruses integrated within a host gene were significantly more frequent in clones of high abundance in vivo than in those with low abundance, but only when integrated in the same transcriptional sense as the host gene. Because of the known mitogenic properties of Tax, we postulated that Tax-expressing clones would reach a higher mean abundance than non-expressing clones in the circulation. But again the results confounded expectation: the frequency of Tax expression was significantly greater in low-abundance clones (Fig. 2) [80]. Although it was unexpected, this result is consistent with the observations noted above that orientation of the provirus in the same transcriptional sense as the flanking host gene is associated with silencing check details of Tax expression [80] and with high clone abundance [72] and [80]. Since the proviral load is higher in HAM/TSP patients than in asymptomatic HTLV-1 carriers, and oligoclonal proliferation is frequently detected more easily in samples

from patients with HAM/TSP [54], it was natural to infer that oligoclonal proliferation was stronger in HAM/TSP and therefore that it might contribute to the pathogenesis of the inflammatory disease. However, this inference could not be formally tested in the absence of an objective measure of oligoclonality. What is required is a measure of the non-uniformity or entropy of the clone frequency distribution. A widely used entropic index, the Shannon index, Rolziracetam is of very limited usefulness here because this index is correlated with the sample size, which can be very large in high-throughput

sequencing. We therefore defined [72] the oligoclonality index (OCI), an application of the Gini index (Fig. 3). An OCI of 1 indicates perfect monoclonality, whereas an index of 0 indicates that each clone has the same frequency. This index allows a rigorous quantitative comparison of the degree of oligoclonality between disease states. We found that, contrary to expectation, there is no significant difference in oligoclonality (as measured by OCI) between patients with HAM/TSP and asymptomatic carriers [72]; The OCI in patients with malignant disease, ATLL, is significantly higher, as expected. Further, the degree of oligoclonality (OCI) does not correlate with the proviral load in patients with non-malignant infection [72]. Rather, the proviral load correlated with the total number of distinct clones, and this number is significantly greater in patients with HAM/TSP than in asymptomatic carriers.

The eccentricity of a worm undergoing an Ω bend (which is roughly

The eccentricity of a worm undergoing an Ω bend (which is roughly circular) is smaller than that of a worm exhibiting forward swimming (which is roughly linear). Ω bends were defined as occurring when the eccentricity fell below a threshold value set by measuring the eccentricity in a large number of manually detected Ω bends. The result for turning rate in each assay was the mean value for three to six animals in the assay. A similar number of adult animals were moved onto either a control plate that contained a fresh lawn of the benign bacterium E. coli OP50 or a training plate that contained a fresh lawn

of the pathogenic bacterium P. aeruginosa PA14 for aversive training for 6 hr at 20°C. Control and training plates were prepared by inoculating 10 cm NGM plates with 0.5 ml overnight NGM culture of OP50 or PA14, respectively, and incubating at 26°C for 1.5 days. Calcium imaging was performed in a microfluidic device essentially as http://www.selleckchem.com/products/Romidepsin-FK228.html described (Chalasani et al., 2007 and Chronis et al., 2007) with minor modifications. Fluorescence time lapse imaging (100 ms exposure) was performed on a Nikon Eclipse Ti-U inverted microscope with a 40× oil immersion objective and a Photometrics CoolSnap EZ camera. The detail of the procedure is included in the Supplemental Information. We thank Caenorhabditis Genetics Center for C. elegans strains; Dr. Cori Bargmann, Dr. Piali Sengupta, Dr. Kyuhyung Kim,

and Harvard Center for Nanoscale Systems for helps with the microfluidics system; Dr. Linjiao Luo and Dr. Mi Zhang for helps with this website the femtosecond laser apparatus; Dr. Edward Soucy and Dr. Joel Greenwood at Center for Brain Science Neuroengineering others Facility at Harvard

University for technical supports; and Dr. Junichi Nakai for the recombinant DNA clone pN1-G-CaMP. We thank Dr. Joshua Sanes, Dr. Cori Bargmann, Dr. Kenneth Blum, Dr. Catherine Dulac, and Zhang laboratory members for thoughtful comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the funding from Howard Hughes Medical Institute (K.S.), NIH grant 4R00NS57931 (D.C.-R.), The Esther A. and Joseph Klingenstein Fund, March of Dimes Foundation, The Alfred P. Sloan Foundation, The John Merck Fund, NIH (Y.Z.) and the McKnight Foundation, NSF, and NIH (A.D.T.S.). Author contributions are as follows: Y.Z. conceived of the study; H.H., M.H., A.D.T.S., and Y.Z. designed experiments; H.H., M.H., Y.S., Y.Q., and Y.Z. performed experiments; C.V.G., C.F-Y., and A.D.T.S. provided experimental and analytical tools; D.C-R. and K.S. contributed to genetic reagents; and H.H., M.H., A.D.T.S., and Y.Z. analyzed data and wrote the article. “
“It has often been proposed that the precise timing and correlation of neuronal activity is as much a part of the neural code as the spatial distribution of spike rate activity in the population (deCharms and Zador, 2000 and Tiesinga et al., 2008).

(2008) Both γ-7 and γ-5 enhance the mean channel conductance and

(2008). Both γ-7 and γ-5 enhance the mean channel conductance and have a modest effect on the rectification

of Glu4 homomers. In striking contrast to Kato et al. (2008), γ-5 Vemurafenib molecular weight was found to preferentially modulate the mean channel conductance of AMPARs composed of “long-form” subunits, which are predominantly GluA2 lacking and calcium permeable (Soto et al., 2009) (Table 1). Further study will be required to reconcile these contradictory findings. Nevertheless, the unique characteristics of type II TARPs add a degree of functional diversity, and possibly bidirectional control, to AMPAR trafficking and gating. TARPs exhibit widespread and extensively overlapping expression patterns throughout the brain as assessed by in situ

hybridization. Type I and II TARPs are found in both neurons and glia and display complex, cell-type-specific expression that varies over the course of development (Tomita et al., 2003, Fukaya et al., 2005 and Lein et al., 2007). Given their apparent functional redundancy, why are there buy PLX-4720 so many TARP family members? Why do some cell types appear to only express one TARP subtype while another expresses a multitude? A great deal can be learned about the subtype-specific role of TARPs in brain function by examining their differential expression patterns and complex effects on AMPAR trafficking and gating following their genetic deletion. A useful way of unpacking these questions is to consider TARP subtype-specific effects in well-characterized cell types in the hippocampus, cerebellum, neocortex, and thalamus (Table 2). Because the expression of synaptic plasticity at Schaffer colateral-CA1 pyramidal neuron synapses depends

on the activity-dependent regulation of postsynaptic AMPARs (Malenka and Bear, 2004 and Kerchner and Nicoll, 2008), a compelling issue since the discovery of TARPs has been discerning their role in modulating AMPAR trafficking and plasticity in these neurons. CA1 pyramidal neurons are known to express multiple TARP family members, including stargazin, γ-3, γ-4, γ7, and γ-8. However, a striking and unique feature of the hippocampus is the selective enrichment of Suplatast tosilate γ-8 (Tomita et al., 2003, Fukaya et al., 2005 and Lein et al., 2007). The generation of the γ-8 knockout (KO) mouse revealed that AMPAR expression and distribution are selectively diminished in the hippocampus, as evidenced by the dramatic reduction in hippocampal GluA subunit protein expression without a corresponding change in amounts of mRNA. At the subcellular level, immunogold electron microscopy showed that both synaptic and extrasynaptic AMPARs are severely diminished. Interestingly, CA1 pyramidal neurons from γ-8 KO mice exhibit relatively modest reductions in field EPSC (fEPSC) slope, AMPA/NMDA ratio, and mEPSC amplitude, but do exhibit the near-complete loss of extrasynaptic AMPARs.

, 2001, Sato

and Schall, 2003 and Thompson et al , 1996)

, 2001, Sato

and Schall, 2003 and Thompson et al., 1996) and these selection signals do not depend on the generation of a saccade (Thompson et al., 1997). Moreover, when the saccade is directed to a stimulus outside the RF, FEF neurons are activated by distracters similar to the target (Bichot and Schall, 1999) confirming that visual I-BET-762 mw selection signals are independent of saccade production signals in the FEF. Finally, electrical microstimulation of the FEF in an antisaccade task demonstrated that covert attention is independent of the actual saccade preparation (Juan et al., 2004). Although the evidence listed above argues against a causal role of saccadic activity in attentional processes, a direct test should include a comparison of the responses of all classes of FEF neurons (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985) in both covert attention and saccade tasks, as well as a comparison of their roles in top-down attentional feedback to visual cortex. Our study now does that. We employed an endogenous attention task and a manual response, to preclude any preparation for a saccade. An earlier study also examined the source of attentional signals among FEF neurons (Thompson et al., 2005). Using a pop-out visual search task that required no saccadic response, the authors showed that only cells with visual responses in the FEF (visual

and visuomovement) modulated their activity with the locus of attention. Saccade-related movement neurons were suppressed in the attention task and this suppression was not spatially selective. Our data on firing rates ABT-199 are in large agreement with Thompson et al. and extend their results in two ways. First, during sustained attention, we found that only purely visual neurons increased their activity with attention to the RF and at this time the activity of movement neurons decreased when attention was directed toward their movement

field. The suppression of saccade-related movement neurons with attention may be the result of local processing within the Astemizole FEF so that saccades are inhibited downstream based on behavioral context. Indeed, SC, which lies closer to the brainstem saccade generator, receives projections mainly from the infragranular layers of the FEF where most movement neurons lie (Fries, 1984, Pouget et al., 2009 and Segraves and Goldberg, 1987). Second, while Thompson et al. used a task characterized by exogenous shifts of attention (pop-out), we used a task that required endogenous shifts of attention. It has been previously suggested that endogenous, rather than exogenous, shifts of attention are mediated by oculomotor processes related to the preparation for a saccade (Awh et al., 2006, Klein, 1980 and Rizzolatti et al., 1994). The two studies together, therefore, demonstrate that neither in exogenous nor in endogenous attention do FEF saccade-related movement neurons contribute to shifts of attention.